Anti-fat bias

Anti-fat bias refers to the prejudicial assumption of personality characteristics based on a visual assessment of a person as suffering from obesity based on physical characteristics. Anti-fat bias can be found in many facets of society, and the media’s portrayal of obese individuals has often been blamed for the pervasiveness of this phenomenon.

Contents

Trait attribution

Anti-fat bias leads individuals to label obese members of society with negative traits such as "lazy", "greedy", "stupid", "smelly", "slow", "unmotivated", and, most of all, "fat." This bias is not restricted only to clinically obese individuals, but also encompasses those whose body shape is in some way found unacceptable (although still within the normal or overweight BMI range).[1]

Anti-fat bias can be moderated by giving a mitigating context to the individual’s appearance of obesity.[2] For example, when told an individual was obese because of "overeating" and "lack of exercise", a higher implicit bias was found among study participants than those told that the individual’s obesity was due to "genetics". The group told that "genetics" were to blame did not exhibit a lowered implicit bias after the explanation, however. Thus, anti-fat bias may result from disgust.

Anti-fat bias is not a strictly Western cultural phenomenon. Instances of implicit anti-fat bias have been found across several cultures.[3]

Newer research suggests that the stereotypical traits and attributions are post hoc justifications for the expression of prejudice against obese people. Initial unconscious feelings of disgust and associated negative valance when viewing obesity my evoke negative emotions that need to be justified, and thus, negative attributions supporting or justifying the negative feelings towards obesity are produced. Additionally, recent work around physical appearance issues, body image, and anti-fat or obesity prejudice suggests that feelings about one's own appearance may stimulate downward physical comparisons with obese individuals in order to make one feel better about their own physical appearance.[4][5]

Media

The media is often blamed for the strong negative trait associations that society has toward overweight individuals. There is a great deal of empirical research to support the idea of Thin Ideal Media, or the idea that the media tends to glorify and focus on thin actors and actresses, models, and other public figures while avoiding the use of overweight individuals.

In a study of children’s movies and books for messages about the importance of appearance, media targeted for children were heavily saturated with messages emphasizing attractiveness as an important part of relationships and interpersonal interaction.[6] Among the movies used in the study, two Disney movies contained the highest amount of messages about personal beauty. This study also found 64% of the videos studied portrayed obese characters as unattractive, evil, cruel, unfriendly, and more than half of the portrayals involved the consideration or consumption of food.

Representation of overweight individuals in prime time programming is not representative of the actual proportion in the population.[7] Only 14% of females and 24% of males featured in the top ten prime-time fictional programs of 2003 were overweight. Those that were shown had few romantic interactions, rarely shared affection with other characters, and were frequently shown consuming food.

In 2007, another analysis sampled 135 scenes featuring overweight individuals from popular television programs and movies and coded for anti-fat humor.[8] The majority of anti-fat humor found was verbal and directed at the individual in their presence.

On September 29, 2011, prominent nationally-syndicated columnist Michael Kinsley (founding editor of Slate magazine) wrote, "New Jersey Governor Chris Christie cannot be president: He is just too fat . . . .why should Christie’s weight be more than we can bear in a president? Why should it even be a legitimate issue if he runs? One reason is that a presidential candidate should be judged on behavior and character . . . . . Perhaps Christie is the one to help us get our national appetites under control. But it would help if he got his own under control first."[9]

Governor Christie's response, on October 4, 2011: "The people who pretend to be serious commentators who wrote about this are among the most ignorant I’ve ever heard in my life. To say that, because you’re overweight, you are therefore undisciplined — you know, I don’t think undisciplined people get to achieve great positions in our society, so that kind of stuff is just ignorant."[10]

Effects

Anti-fat bias can be found in the educational system. When compared to a group of psychology students of the same age, a group of students training to become physical education (PE) teachers were more likely to display implicit anti-fat attitudes.[11]

One dangerous effect of anti-fat bias is the presence of this bias in healthcare professionals, whose biases could result in a lower quality of treatment for overweight patients. Even those medical professionals who specialize in the treatment of obesity have been found to have strong negative associations toward obese individuals.[12]

Anti-fat bias can also be found at an early age. Preschool-aged children reported a preference for average-sized children over overweight children as friends.[13] As a consequence of anti-fat bias, overweight individuals often find themselves suffering repercussions in many facets of society, including legal and employment issues.[14]

Unfortunately it also appears that prejudice or stigma interventions seeking to reduce anti-fat or obesity prejudice are largely ineffective.[15]

References

  1. ^ Lerner, R.; Gellert, E. (1969). "Body build identification, preference and aversion in children". Developmental Psychology 1 (5): 456–462. doi:10.1037/h0027966. 
  2. ^ Teachman, B.A.; Gapinski, K.D.; Brownell, K.D.; Rawlins, M.; Jeyaram, S. (2003). "Demonstrations of implicit anti-fat bias: The impact of providing causal information and evoking empathy". Health Psychology 22 (1): 68–78. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.22.1.68. PMID 12558204. 
  3. ^ Crandall, C.; D’Anello, S.; Sakalli, N.; Lazarus, E.; Nejtardt, G.; Feather, N. (2001). "An attribution-model of prejudice: Anti-fat attitudes in six nations". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 21 (1): 30–37. 
  4. ^ O’Brien KS, Hunter JA, Halberstadt J, Anderson, J. Body image and explicit and implicit anti-fat attitudes: The mediating role of physical appearance comparisons. Body Image 2007; 4: 249-256.
  5. ^ O’Brien KS, Caputi P, Minto R, Peoples G, Hooper C, Kell S et al. Upward and Downward Physical Appearance-Related Comparisons: Development of a Measure and Examination of Predictive Qualities. Body Image 2009; 6: 201-206.
  6. ^ Herbozo, S.; Tantleff-Dunn, S.; Gokee-Larose, J.; Thompson, J.K. (2004). "Beauty and thinness messages in children's media: A content analysis". Eating Disorders 12: 21–34. 
  7. ^ Greenberg, B.; Eastin, M.; Hofschire, L.; Lachlan, K.; Brownell, K. (2003). "Portrayals of Overweight and Obese Individuals on Commercial Television". American Journal of Public Health 93 (8): 1342–1348. doi:10.2105/AJPH.93.8.1342. PMC 1447967. PMID 12893625. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=1447967. 
  8. ^ Himes, S.M.; Thompson, J.K. (2007). "Fat stigmatization in television shows and movies: A content analysis". Obesity 15 (3): 712–719. doi:10.1038/oby.2007.635. 
  9. ^ Kinsley, Michael (September 29, 2011). "Requiem for a Governor Before He’s in the Ring: Michael Kinsley". Bloomberg View. http://www.bloomberg.com/news/2011-09-30/requiem-for-a-governor-before-he-s-in-the-ring-michael-kinsley.html. Retrieved 2011-10-06. 
  10. ^ Christie, Chris (October 4, 2011). "Pundits Pack Meaner Punch Than Comedians’ Fat Jokes". New York Times. http://www.nytimes.com/2011/10/05/us/politics/christie-says-jokes-are-ok-but-not-political-debate-on-weight.html. Retrieved 2011-10-06. 
  11. ^ O’Brien, K.S.; Hunter, J.A.; Banks, M. (2007). "Implicit anti-fat bias in physical educators: Physical attributes, ideology and socialization". International Journal of Obesity 31 (2): 308–314. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0803398. PMID 16733526. 
  12. ^ Teachman, B.A.; Brownell, K.D. (2001). "Implicit anti-fat bias among health professionals: Is anyone immune?". International Journal of Obesity 25 (10): 1525–1531. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0801745. PMID 11673776. 
  13. ^ Musher-Eizenman, D.; Holub, S.; Miller, A.; Goldstein, S.; Edwards-Leeper, L. (2004). "Body size stigmatization in preschool children: The role of control attributions". Journal of Pediatric Psychology 29 (8): 613–620. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsh063. PMID 15491983. 
  14. ^ Puhl, R.; Brownell, K. (2001). "Bias, discrimination, and obesity". Obesity Research 9 (12): 788–805. doi:10.1038/oby.2001.108. PMID 11743063. 
  15. ^ Daníelsdóttir S, O’Brien KS, Ciao A. Anti-fat prejudice reduction: A review of published studies. Obesity Facts 2010; 3: 47–58.